At the output end, we find not only various sets of body muscles that we can voluntarily control, in the trunk, limbs, eyes, and tongue, but also sets that subserve the less voluntary or involuntary housekeeping functions, such as making our stomachs churn, our water pass or bowels move, and our sphincters (between these events) hold orifices closed. We also need to qualify our model with respect to direction of information flow. The prevailing direction in our diagram is obviously from left to right, from input to output, but in almost every case in which information is transferred from one stage to the next, reciprocal connections feed information back from the second stage to the first. (We can sometimes guess what such feedback might be useful for, but in almost no case do we have incisive understanding.) Finally, even within a given stage we often find a rich network of connections between neighboring cells of the same order. Thus to say that a structure contains a specific number of stages is almost always an oversimplification. When I began working in neurology in the early 1950s, this basic plan of the nervous system was well understood. But in those days no one had any clear idea how to interpret this bucket-brigade-like handing on of information from one stage to the next. Today we know far more about the ways in which the information is transformed in some parts of the brain; in other parts we still know almost nothing. The remaining chapters of this text are devoted to the visual system, the one we understand best today. I will next try to give a preview of a few of the things we know about that system.